The ability to imitate is an important cognitive ability in young children, and leads to symbolic play. These skills develop in stages and are related to the development of important foundational skills in other developmental areas, such as social and emotional development, language development, and cognitive flexibility and creativity.
THE ABILITY TO IMITATE IS AN IMPORTANT COGNITIVE ABILITY IN YOUNG CHILDREN, AND LEADS TO SYMBOLIC PLAY. THESE SKILLS DEVELOP IN STAGES AND ARE RELATED TO IMPORTANT FOUNDATIONAL SKILLS IN OTHER DEVELOPMENTAL AREAS, SUCH AS SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT, LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT, AND COGNITIVE FLEXIBILITY AND CREATIVITY.
Children's ability to imitate (i.e. voluntarily match the actions of others) is an important mechanism for social learning and acquiring new knowledge.
In one study, newborns as young as 42 minutes old were found to match gestures shown to them, including tongue protrusion and mouth opening.
However, more recent studies have also found that babies learnt how to imitate only within their first month of birth. In a study of infants at one, two, six, and nine weeks of age, researchers found that they did not imitate any of the behaviours they observed. In response to the grownups they saw, they were just as likely to produce a different gesture as they were to produce a matching one.
The findings now suggest that imitation is not an innate behaviour but one that is learnt in babies' first months. One possibility is that babies learn to imitate other people by watching other people imitate them. Another study from the same lab found that parents imitate their babies once every two minutes on average. Hence, this could be a powerful means by which infants learn to link their gestures with those of another person.
Imitation skills enable babies to share attention (joint-attention skills) on an event, which builds cognitive skills. Babies should be able to imitate what an adult does by 6 to 8 months. Studies have found that infants who are later diagnosed with autism experience considerable delays in developing imitation and play skills as early as 9 to 12 months of age, and that although these skills improve over time, they do so at a slow rate.
Moving from imitation to symbolic play: Play starts “primitively” as sensorimotor play before one to two years, with an exploration of properties and functions of objects and imitation. Towards the end of this stage, children may imitate actions they have observed in adults, like bringing a toy cup to their mouths, but most of the time, objects are still used for their intended purposes.
Between the ages of one and two, symbolic or pretend play replaces sensorimotor play. True symbolic thought emerges around 18 months of age with children’s ability to think in images and symbols.
Symbolic play, also known as pretend play, is the ability to use objects, actions or ideas to represent other objects, actions, or ideas during play. Before the symbolic play level of development, a box is a box. After a child has developed symbolic play abilities, a box can be a phone or anything the child imagines it to be. Infants reach the pre-symbolic level between eight and eleven months of age, and the first milestone of symbolic play is typically evident at around eleven to twelve months of age. Orr, E., & Geva, R. (2015). Symbolic play and language development. Infant Behaviour and Development, 38, 147–161. Scott, H. K., & Cogburn, M. (2019) Peer Play. In StatPearls (Internet). StatPearls Publishing. Retrieved from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK513223/
Symbolic play has sparked the interest of cognitive scientists because of its potential importance in children’s cognitive development, such as the development of cognitive flexibility and creativity. Orr, E., & Geva, R. (2015). Symbolic play and language development. Infant Behaviour and Development, 38, 147–161. Quinn, S., Donnelly, S., & Kidd, E. (2018). The relationship between symbolic play and language acquisition: a meta-analytic review. Developmental Review, 49, 121–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.05.005
Researchers have noted correlations between various forms of symbolic play and children’s understanding of others’ minds.
In the contents of symbolic play, a child reflects different social situations, such as family relationships, shopping, and working adults, in a creative way. This contributes to the adoption of gender roles, learning the rules, socialisation, mastering the child's culture, understanding and adopting some higher emotions, overcoming egocentrism, and moving away from the present situation.
Studies have found that early pretend play predicts later divergent thinking and may correlate to later academic performance. A four-year longitudinal study of 61 students in a private school for girls in kindergarten through fourth grade found that pretend play predicted more divergent thinking in the children. Children whose pretend play was more imaginative and organised generated more alternate uses for common objects. Positive affect in play predicted originality of responses. In addition, both pretend play and divergent thinking predicted girls’ mathematics achievement longitudinally.